Preface | p. ix |
Introduction: Of chimps, children, and grown-ups | p. 1 |
Language as a defining human attribute | p. 3 |
Diverse semiotic systems | p. 3 |
The uniqueness of human language | p. 8 |
Native language as childhood language | p. 11 |
Notes | p. 15 |
Nature and nurture: The poverty of the stimulus | p. 18 |
Language and nurture: The contribution of the environment | p. 18 |
Language and human nature: Phenotype from genotype | p. 22 |
Human nature and nature: A deep parallel between ontogenesis and speciation | p. 28 |
Notes | p. 31 |
Human nature and culture: Concept labeling | p. 34 |
Language and thought: Speechless rational animals | p. 34 |
The conceptual system: A common heritage of apes and humans? | p. 35 |
Increasing vocabularies: Label learning | p. 38 |
"A rose by any other name ...": Saussurean arbitrariness | p. 38 |
Numbers and colors versus smells | p. 40 |
Unlabeled and labeled inborn concepts | p. 45 |
A parallel with the immune system | p. 46 |
Genetically programmed semantic fields | p. 47 |
Notes | p. 51 |
Culture and language: Civilization and its progress | p. 55 |
Knowing and doing: Diverging humanities and postmodern humanists | p. 55 |
Interpretation and translation: Not just indeterminacy | p. 59 |
Semantic connections: Truths of meaning | p. 60 |
New words and enriched notions (with an excursus on Newspeak) | p. 64 |
Intertranslatability and cultural wealth: Levels of understanding | p. 68 |
Semantic roles: Conceptual and linguistic structure at the interface | p. 74 |
Notes | p. 78 |
Language explanation: The growth of a child's mind | p. 81 |
Some elementary properties of human language | p. 81 |
The creative aspect of language use: Descartes's Problem | p. 82 |
The recursive property of linguistic knowledge: Humboldt's Problem | p. 85 |
The logical problem of knowledge acquisition: Plato's Problem | p. 86 |
Chomsky's solution: The development of transformational generative grammar | p. 87 |
The first thirty years (1949-79): An updated revival of a classical tradition | p. 90 |
The radical departure of 1980: A principles-and-parameters framework | p. 94 |
Notes | p. 97 |
Linguistic knowledge: Universals and particulars | p. 100 |
Language invariance: Part of the genetic endowment | p. 100 |
Phrase structure: Two-level binary branching | p. 100 |
Transformations: Structure-dependent operations | p. 114 |
Language variation: The newborn's questionnaire | p. 119 |
Morphology as paradigmatic grammar | p. 120 |
The syntagmatic grammar and its interfaces | p. 125 |
Notes | p. 126 |
Language maturation and beyond: A new look at the Critical Period Hypothesis | p. 130 |
Language and puberty: A parallel between linguistic maturity and sexual maturity | p. 130 |
From Skinner to Chomsky: Lenneberg's Problem | p. 133 |
An embryological solution: Blame the brain | p. 137 |
The active language organ: Lifelong innate knowledge | p. 139 |
Unsettable switches: Lifelong patchwork | p. 143 |
Other hypotheses | p. 156 |
Notes | p. 161 |
Language research and language teaching: A renewed promise | p. 172 |
Language theory and methods of instruction | p. 173 |
Scientific grammars and pedagogic grammars | p. 177 |
A range of options for language programs | p. 183 |
Notes | p. 192 |
Overview: New prospects for the study of language | p. 198 |
Baby or beast: Human language and other semiotic systems | p. 198 |
Child or grown-up: Language universals and language particulars | p. 203 |
Design or randomness: A new kind of language program | p. 204 |
Conclusion: A modest proposal | p. 211 |
Notes | p. 214 |
Bibliography | p. 218 |
Index | p. 267 |
Table of Contents provided by Syndetics. All Rights Reserved. |