Preface | p. xi |
Acknowledgements | p. xv |
Emotion since Darwin | p. 1 |
What is an emotion? | p. 1 |
A biological approach to emotion? | p. 5 |
Darwin and 'the expression of the emotions in animals and man' | p. 7 |
Cannon and the 'utility of the bodily changes in pain and great emotions' | p. 8 |
James and feelings as the basis for emotion | p. 9 |
Development of the emotions | p. 11 |
Learning and emotion | p. 12 |
Cognition and emotion | p. 13 |
Biology and emotion | p. 13 |
Releasers and state-dependent reflexes | p. 17 |
Apologia | p. 17 |
Neuroscience and emotion--a brief digression | p. 17 |
Neurophysiology and emotion | p. 18 |
Releasers and emotion | p. 21 |
Reflexes and goal-directedness | p. 22 |
Electrical stimulation of the brain and goal-directed behaviour | p. 24 |
Electrical excitation versus natural excitability | p. 28 |
The neural basis for the release of behaviour | p. 28 |
Releasers, state-dependent reflexes and emotion | p. 31 |
Purpose and emotion | p. 33 |
Teleonomy, a redefinition of purpose | p. 33 |
Teleonomy versus teleology | p. 34 |
Emotions and teleonomy | p. 36 |
Teleonomy and its implication for a unitary view of emotion | p. 38 |
Expression: a window on the emotions? | p. 40 |
Why do emotions produce expressions | p. 40 |
Are human expressions innate or acquired? | p. 42 |
The description of expressions | p. 45 |
Identical forms of expression with different teleonomy | p. 47 |
Is there a one-to-one link between an expression and an emotion? | p. 48 |
Are physiological changes epiphenomena of emotion? | p. 51 |
Why do physiological changes accompany emotion? | p. 51 |
Could physiological changes play a role in emotion? | p. 52 |
Autonomic and hormonal discharge in emotion | p. 53 |
Emotional feeling after the elimination of peripheral feedback | p. 55 |
Do visceral reactions differ in different emotions? | p. 57 |
Emotion-specific release of hormones | p. 57 |
Emotion-specific changes in autonomic response | p. 59 |
Do visceral reactions determine what emotion is reported? | p. 62 |
Conclusion | p. 65 |
Somatic influences on the emotions | p. 67 |
Do physiological changes determine emotion? | p. 67 |
Effects of peripheral sympathectomy | p. 67 |
Non-surgical manipulation of the sympathetic system | p. 69 |
Emotion after the removal of hormonal influences | p. 70 |
Secretions of the pituitary-adrenal cortex as controllers of emotion | p. 72 |
False feedback experiments | p. 72 |
A role for heart rate changes | p. 74 |
Conclusion | p. 75 |
Optimal foraging and the partial reinforcement effect: a model for the teleonomy of feelings? | p. 77 |
Teleonomy, physiological change and feeling | p. 77 |
How can you assess teleonomy of behaviour? | p. 78 |
Optimality and the partial reinforcement extinction effect (PREE) | p. 79 |
Omission of reward and the generation of frustration | p. 81 |
Accounts of the PREE in terms of frustration | p. 83 |
Simple associative accounts of the PREE | p. 84 |
Attention, aftereffects, toughening up and frustration | p. 87 |
Behavioural separation of after-effects, stimuli of frustration and toughening up | p. 87 |
Pharmacological separation of after-effects, stimuli of frustration and toughening up | p. 88 |
The teleonomy of feelings | p. 90 |
Do emotions mature or differentiate? | p. 93 |
Teleonomy and procreation | p. 93 |
Emotional development and emotional maturation | p. 94 |
Do innate emotional expressions imply innate emotions? | p. 95 |
Immediate effects of separation from parents | p. 96 |
Distinct emotional reactions in the neonate | p. 97 |
The effects of perinatal manipulations on adult behaviour | p. 100 |
Long-term effects of early environment on adult behaviour | p. 101 |
Do emotions mature and differentiate? | p. 103 |
Cognition, learning and emotion | p. 105 |
What place has learning in the analysis of emotion? | p. 105 |
Radical behaviourism and cognitive learning theory | p. 107 |
Limitations on the valid use of cognitive terms in learning theory | p. 109 |
The use of emotion words without colloquial implications | p. 111 |
The development of two-process theories of learning | p. 111 |
Emotion and the release of species-specific behaviour | p. 115 |
Emotion as an antidote to motivation | p. 118 |
Motivation versus emotion | p. 121 |
Emotion as a critical aspect of instrumental conditioning | p. 124 |
Conclusions | p. 128 |
Interaction of the components of emotion | p. 130 |
Dialectical and non-dialectical interactions in emotion | p. 130 |
The influence of the face on emotions | p. 132 |
Facial expressiveness as a personality characteristic | p. 135 |
Autonomic reactions and achievement | p. 137 |
Hormone-behaviour interactions | p. 138 |
Invisible interactions | p. 140 |
Some implications for the study of emotion | p. 140 |
Of mice and men | p. 143 |
From mouse to monkey to man | p. 143 |
The lachrymose ape | p. 145 |
Separation distress--a general emotion? | p. 148 |
Where do the innate human expressions come from? | p. 150 |
Effects of electrical stimulation of the brain in humans | p. 153 |
If rats are like little furry men, are mice like diminutive rats? | p. 155 |
Is there frustration in rats and humans? | p. 159 |
Cognition and corticosteroids | p. 162 |
Emotionality in rat and man | p. 164 |
Conclusion | p. 167 |
Biology and emotion: some conclusions | p. 169 |
What is an emotion? | p. 169 |
A biological approach to emotion? | p. 173 |
Specification of emotions | p. 175 |
Darwin as the father of the psychology of emotion | p. 175 |
Emotion, drive and state-dependent reflexes | p. 176 |
Teleonomy and emotion | p. 178 |
Comparison of the components of emotional reaction | p. 179 |
Development, learning and emotion | p. 181 |
The way forward | p. 182 |
A specific example | p. 185 |
Envoi | p. 191 |
Glossary | p. 192 |
Notes | p. 196 |
References | p. 204 |
Index | p. 219 |
Table of Contents provided by Syndetics. All Rights Reserved. |